History of canids in Chile and impacts on prey adaptations
Benjamín Silva Rochefort1 & Meredith Root-Bernstein2,3,4
  1. Facultad de Ciencias Biológicas, Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile
  2. Center of Applied Ecology and Sustainability, Santiago, Chile
  3. Institute of Ecology and Biodiversity, Santiago, Chile
  4. UMR CESCO, CNRS, Musée National d’Histoire Naturelle, Paris, France
Abstract
Artiodactyl prey species of Chile, especially guanacos (Lama guanicoe ) are reported to be very susceptible to predation by pack hunting feral dogs. It has been previously suggested that guanacos and endemic South American deer may have evolved in the absence of pack-hunting cursorial predators. However, the paleoecology of canid presence in southern South America and Chile is unclear. Here, we review the literature on South American and Chilean canids, their distributions, ecologies and hunting behaviour. We consider both wild and domestic canids, including Canis familiaris breeds. We establish two known antipredator defense behaviours of guanacos: predator inspection of ambush predators, e.g. Puma concolor , and rushing at and kicking smaller cursorial predators, e.g. Lycalopex culpaeus . We propose that since the late Pleistocene extinction of hypercarnivorous group-hunting canids east of the Andes, there were no native species creating group-hunting predation pressures on guanacos. Endemic deer of Chile may have never experienced group hunting selection pressure from native predators. Even hunting dogs (or other canids) used by indigenous groups in the far north and extreme south of Chile (and presumably the center as well) appear to have been used primarily within ambush hunting strategies. This may account for the susceptibility of guanacos and other prey species to feral dog attacks. We detail seven separate hypotheses that require further investigation in order to assess how best to respond to the threat posed by feral dogs to the conservation of native deer and camelids in Chile and other parts of South America.
Keywords: anti-predator defense; prehispanic dogs; cursorial; ambush; hunting; ethnography; evolution of prey behavior; predation strategy
Introduction
The damage to biodiversity caused by feral dogs (Canis familiaris) is well-established globally. Young et al. (2011), in a global review of feral dog impacts, show that the negative effects of C. familiaris on native species is widespread. Feral dogs are a serious problem for conservation, mainly because of their predation on native species, transmission of diseases, and competition with other predators, which may destabilize ecosystems (Ritchie et al. 2014; Hughes & Macdonald 2013). For example, on the Galapagos Islands a significant decline in marine iguanas (Amblyrhinchus cristatus) resulted from the establishment of dogs on the island (Kruuk & Snell 1981). Feral and freely ranging dogs in Brazilian protected areas displace native predators, spread diseases, and predate small and medium sized wildlife (Lessa et al. 2016). South America is a hotspot of C. familiaris negative impacts on threatened species (Doherty et al. 2017). Why exactly are dogs such a destabilizing conservation threat in South America in particular? Geist (1998) partly addressed one angle of this question when he concluded that the South American deer are not adapted to cursorial predators like dogs, and that deer traits like body size, locomotion, and reproductive investment, imply that there must have been only ambush predators present during the radiation of the South American deer species. In this paper, we re-examine and extend this hypothesis, to ask whether it can explain why deer and also camelids of South America are so vulnerable to feral dogs. We look more completely at all the evidence for the hunting strategies of prehistoric predators as well as domesticated canids associated with Precolumbian human presence. We focus on the case of Chile, and we end our review by evaluating the state of knowledge and suggesting hypotheses that need to be researched in order to better understand how the evolution and ecology of predator-prey interactions affect possible responses to the conservation challenge posed by feral dogs.
In Chile, feral dogs represent economic as well as environmental damage. C. familiaris is estimated to cause 57,000 sheep deaths annually (Bonacic & Muñoz 2014), although it should be noted that this includes deaths caused by both feral and domestic dogs allowed to freely roam agricultural areas. Both feral and freely ranging human-dependent dogs cause significant reductions in the populations of threatened species in Chile. Freely ranging dogs that are poorly fed are more likely to prey on and harass wildlife (Silva-Rodríguez & Sieving 2011). Rural dogs are interference competitors with the native fox Lycalopex griseus (Silva-Rodríguez et al. 2010), eat the eggs of migratory birds that nest in Chile (Shuttler et al. 2009), and predate on native deer species, including pudu Pudu puda (Silva-Rodríguez et al. 2009; Silva-Rodríguez & Sieving 2012), and huemul Hippocamelus bisulcus (Flueck & Smith-Flueck 2006; Corti et al. 2010). Reports from Peru (Barrio 2007) state that fawns of taruka, Hippocamelus antisensis, a deer also found in northern Chile, may be killed by shepherd and feral dogs. The native camelids, principally guanaco (Lama guanicoe), are considered by experts (pers. comm. WCS Chile Grupo Núcleo Guanacos 2020) to be particularly susceptible to being killed by feral dogs, and dog presence is considered to be a factor preventing the conservation and reintroduction of Lama guanicoe in large parts of its historical range (Farías et al. 2010; González 2010). According to McLaren et al. (2018), vicuña (Vicugna vicugna ), a high-altitude relative of the guanaco, are hunted and harassed by humans with dogs, but feral dogs primarily predate on young offspring. It should be noted that the extent of the threat to species posed by dogs in Chile is partly due to the lack of a legal or cultural basis for their control, lethal or otherwise.
However, there are also behavioural and evolutionary features of predators to consider when trying to understand why feral dogs present a large and difficult-to-control threat to Chilean wildlife. When understanding predation, there are two key factors to consider: the predator’s niche, and its hunting strategy. In terms of their niches, in general most canids (Carnivora: Canidae) are omnivores that do not kill (but may scavenge) prey larger than themselves (Sheldon 1992). Small canids < 6 kg are solitary foragers, canids 6-13 kg are facultative cooperative foragers, and larger ones are obligate cooperative foragers (Sheldon 1992) likely to be specialized as carnivores, with some exceptions (Wang et a. 2004). Canids, like other predators, may use either cursorial (pursuit) or ambush strategies. In cursorial strategies, the predator relies on its running adaptations and stamina to exhaust the prey, and may lack specific killing tactics, rather relying on bringing the prey down by biting (Fox 1987; Sheldon 1992; González 2010). In ambush strategies, the predator stalks the prey, remaining undetected until it can pounce on or rush at the prey from a short distance; it may rely on specialized killing tactics (Fox 1987; Sheldon 1992). For example, puma (Puma concolor ) are ambush predators that kill with a specialized crushing bite to the head (Sarno et al. 1999).
In turn, prey species have adaptations such as scanning for predators, alarm calls, escape tactics, group defence tactics, and so on, which may be suited for different predator body sizes, group sizes, and either pursuit or ambush predation strategies (see Caro et al. 2004). Essentially, to escape cursorial predators, prey need to outrun or lose them. By contrast, the advantage of ambush predators is their ability to take a targeted prey individual by surprise from a short distance. An effective defence against ambush predation is “predator inspection” which consists of staring at, approaching, surrounding and/or following the predator so that it cannot gain this advantage (FitzGibbon 1994 provides an excellent review). In a study of gazelle, FitzGibbon (1994) found that they were much more likely to inspect ambush than cursorial predators. Inspecting cursorial predators is likely to be risky and maladaptive, as by shortening the distance to the predator it may increase the chances of being overtaken.
In its feral state, C. familiaris hunts in packs using a cursorial strategy (Farías et al. 2010; Ritchie et al. 2014). Geist (1998) notes that all of the South American deer lack appropriate defenses against what he calls “culling” predators, by which he appears to mean a cursorial strategy where one or more predators picks out and runs down its prey. Geist notes that South American deer are “curious” which suggests they approach possible threats. They also respond too late to attacks, are saltatorial bounders rather than runners, and lack stamina (Geist 1998). It has also been suggested that guanaco lack adaptations to escape cursorial pack hunters (Root-Bernstein & Svenning 2016). The guanaco demonstrates group vigilance and predator inspection, suitable for countering its main native predator, the puma (P. concolor), which hunts by solitary ambush (Darwin 2015 [1839]; Marino & Baldi 2008; pers. obs. MR-B). It is not entirely clear whether guanacos inspect dog packs, but González (2010) attributes high dog-caused mortality to guanacos’ short bursts of fast flight, leaving them unable to outrun dogs with greater stamina. Oyama (2006) describes vicuña (V. vicugna ) in the Peruvian Andes as approaching possible threats such as dogs, and facing predators while giving an alarm call. Similarly to the case of guanaco, vicuñas’ main predators, including puma and foxes, hide in the long grass and ambush them.
If South American deer and camelids lack appropriate defense behaviours, does this imply that this is an evolutionary problem—that they lack an adaptive trait? Although prey must respond differently and appropriately to different predators, this does not necessarily imply the evolution of completely independent responses to each predator. Lind and Cresswell (2005) make the valuable point that “antipredation behaviours are a composite of many behaviours that an animal can adjust…” (p. 945). Components of anti-predator behaviour can be lost under loss of predation pressure, but the presence of any kind of predator may be enough to retain a whole suite of antipredation behaviours for tens of thousands of years (Blumstein 2006). So, to assess the hypothesis that key native Chilean prey species—camelids and deer—lack evolutionary adaptations to cursorial group predation, we would need to establish that they never in the past experienced selection pressures in the form of cursorial group hunting predators. Thus we need to understand when cursorial group-hunting canids first appeared in the southern cone of South America, which is not entirely clear. We focus on canids simply because there are not, and have never been, any group-hunting or cursorial felids in South America.
It is clear that European conquistadors brought domestic dogs to the Americas starting around 500 years ago. However, the history of canids in South America, and ecological and behavioural descriptions of any kinds of prehispanic South American canids, have not been synthesized in a way accessible to conservationists. Here, we review existing evidence for the presence of prehispanic canids and their behaviours and ecological roles, focusing on Chile and the southern cone of South America.
Canids and their hunting ecology