REFERENCES
Allen, J. J., Iacono, W. G., & Danielson, K. D. (1992). The
identification of concealed memories using the event‐related potential
and implicit behavioral measures: A methodology for prediction in the
face of individual differences. Psychophysiology, 29 (5), 504-522.
Ben-Shakhar, G. (2012). Current research and potential applications of
the concealed information test: an overview. Frontiers in
psychology, 3 , 342.
Ben-Shakhar, G., & Nahari, T. (2018). The external validity of studies
examining the detection of concealed knowledge using the Concealed
Information Test. Detecting concealed information and deception ,
59-76.
Cattell, R. B. (1966). The scree test for the number of
factors. Multivariate behavioral research , 1 (2), 245-276.
Daffner, K. R., Alperin, B. R., Mott, K. K., Tusch, E. S., & Holcomb,
P. J. (2015). Age-related differences in early novelty processing: Using
PCA to parse the overlapping anterior P2 and N2 components. Biological
psychology, 105, 83-94.
Dien, J. (2010). The ERP PCA Toolkit: An open source program for
advanced statistical analysis of event-related potential
data. Journal of neuroscience methods , 187 (1), 138-145.
Dien, J., Frishkoff, G. A., & Handy, T. (2005). Event-related
potentials: a methods handbook.
Dien, J., Khoe, W., & Mangun, G. R. (2007). Evaluation of PCA and ICA
of simulated ERPs: Promax vs. Infomax rotations. Human brain
mapping , 28 (8), 742-763.
Donkers, F. C., & Van Boxtel, G. J. (2004). The N2 in go/no-go tasks
reflects conflict monitoring not response inhibition. Brain and
cognition , 56 (2), 165-176.
Ellwanger, J., Rosenfeld, J. P., Sweet, J. J., & Bhatt, M. (1996).
Detecting simulated amnesia for autobiographical and recently learned
information using the P300 event-related potential. International
Journal of Psychophysiology, 23 (1-2), 9-23.
Enriquez-Geppert, S., Konrad, C., Pantev, C., & Huster, R. J. (2010).
Conflict and inhibition differentially affect the N200/P300 complex in a
combined go/nogo and stop-signal task. Neuroimage , 51 (2),
877-887.
Farwell, L. A., & Donchin, E. (1991). The truth will out: Interrogative
polygraphy (“lie detection”) with event‐related brain potentials.Psychophysiology, 28 (5), 531-547.
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A, & Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical
power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlations and regression
analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41 (4), 1149-1160.
Foti, D., Weinberg, A., Dien, J., & Hajcak, G. (2011). Event‐related
potential activity in the basal ganglia differentiates rewards from
nonrewards: Temporospatial principal components analysis and source
localization of the feedback negativity. Human brain mapping ,32 (12), 2207-2216.
Gamer, M., & Berti, S. (2010). Task relevance and recognition of
concealed information have different influences on electrodermal
activity and event‐related brain potentials. Psychophysiology,
47 (2), 355-364.
Ganis, G., Bridges, D., Hsu, C.-W., & Schendan, H. E. (2016). Is
anterior N2 enhancement a reliable electrophysiological index of
concealed information? Neuroimage, 143 , 152-165.
Gronau, N., Ben-Shakhar, G., & Cohen, A. (2005). Behavioral and
physiological measures in the detection of concealed information.Journal of Applied Psychology, 90 (1), 147.
Hajcak, G., Holroyd, C. B., Moser, J. S., & Simons, R. F. (2005). Brain
potentials associated with expected and unexpected good and bad
outcomes. Psychophysiology, 42 (2), 161-170.
Hu, X., Pornpattananangkul, N., & Rosenfeld, J. P. (2013). N200 and
P300 as orthogonal and integrable indicators of distinct awareness and
recognition processes in memory detection. Psychophysiology,
50 (5), 454-464.
Hu, X., & Rosenfeld, J. P. (2012). Combining the P300‐complex
trial‐based Concealed Information Test and the reaction time‐based
autobiographical Implicit Association Test in concealed memory
detection. Psychophysiology, 49 (8), 1090-1100.
Hu, X., Wu, H., & Fu, G. (2011). Temporal course of executive control
when lying about self-and other-referential information: an ERP study.Brain research, 1369 , 149-157.
Jeffreys, H. (1998). The theory of probability . OuP Oxford.
Johnson, R. (1986). A triarchic model of P300 amplitude.Psychophysiology, 23 (4), 367-384.
Kayser, J., Tenke, C. E., & Bruder, G. E. (1998). Dissociation of brain
ERP topographies for tonal and phonetic oddball tasks. Psychophysiology,
35(5), 576-590.
Kim, S. C., Kim, H., Lee, K. E., Song, I., Chang, E. H., Kim, S., &
Kim, H. T. (2022). Retroactive memory interference reduces false
positive outcomes of informed innocents in the P300-based concealed
information test. International Journal of
Psychophysiology , 173 , 9-19.
Kubo, K., & Nittono, H. (2009). The role of intention to conceal in the
P300-based concealed information test. Applied Psychophysiology
and Biofeedback, 34 (3), 227-235.
Linden, D. E. (2005). The P300: Where in the Brain Is It Produced and
What Does It Tell Us? The Neuroscientist, 11 (6), 563-576.
Lu, Y., Rosenfeld, J. P., Deng, X., Zhang, E., Zheng, H., Yan, G., . . .
Hayat, S. Z. (2018). Inferior detection of information from
collaborative versus individual crimes based on a P300 Concealed
Information Test. Psychophysiology, 55 (4), e13021.
Luo, Y. J., Sun, S. Y., Mai, X. Q., Gu, R. L., & Zhang, H. J. (2011).
Outcome evaluation in decision making: ERP studies. Culture and
neural frames of cognition and communication , 249-285.
Lukács, G., Weiss, B., Dalos, V. D., Kilencz, T., Tudja, S., &
Csifcsák, G. (2016). The first independent study on the complex trial
protocol version of the P300-based concealed information test:
Corroboration of previous findings and highlights on vulnerabilities.International Journal of Psychophysiology, 110 , 56-65.
Kass, R. E., & Raftery, A. E. (1995). Bayes factors. Journal of
the american statistical association , 90 (430), 773-795.
klein Selle, N., Gueta, C., Harpaz, Y., Deouell, L. Y., & Ben-Shakhar,
G. (2021). Brain-based concealed memory detection is driven mainly by
orientation to salient items. cortex , 136 , 41-55.
Matsuda, I., & Nittono, H. (2018). A concealment-specific frontal
negative slow wave is generated from the right prefrontal cortex in the
concealed information test. Biological psychology, 135 , 194-203.
Matsuda, I., Nittono, H., Hirota, A., Ogawa, T., & Takasawa, N. (2009).
Event-related brain potentials during the standard autonomic-based
concealed information test. International Journal of
Psychophysiology, 74 (1), 58-68.
Meijer, E. H., Smulders, F. T., Merckelbach, H. L., & Wolf, A. G.
(2007). The P300 is sensitive to concealed face recognition.International Journal of Psychophysiology, 66 (3), 231-237.
Meixner, J. B., & Rosenfeld, J. P. (2011). A mock terrorism application
of the P300‐based concealed information test. Psychophysiology,
48 (2), 149-154.
Meixner, J. B., & Rosenfeld, J. P. (2014). Detecting knowledge of
incidentally acquired, real-world memories using a P300-based
concealed-information test. Psychological Science, 25 (11),
1994-2005.
Noordraven, E., & Verschuere, B. (2013). Predicting the sensitivity of
the reaction time‐based Concealed Information Test. Applied
Cognitive Psychology, 27 (3), 328-335.
Olson, J., Rosenfeld, J. P., Kim, T., & Perrault, E. (2018). The effect
of countermeasures against the reaction time based concealed information
test on the P300 index of knowledge recognition: A combined RT and
P300-based test. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 134 ,
9-14.
Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300: An Integrative Theory of P3a and P3b.Clinical Neurophysiology, 118 (10), 2128-2148.
Proudfit, G. H. (2015). The reward positivity: From basic research on
reward to a biomarker for
depression. Psychophysiology , 52 (4), 449-459.
Rosenfeld, J. P. (2005). Brain fingerprinting: A critical analysis.The Scientific Review of Mental Health Practice, 4 (1), 20-37.
Rosenfeld, J. P. (2011). P300 in detecting concealed information.Memory detection: Theory and application of the Concealed
Information Test , 63-89.
Rosenfeld, J. P. (2019). P300 in detecting concealed information and
deception: A review. Psychophysiology , e13362.
Rosenfeld, J. P., Biroschak, J. R., & Furedy, J. J. (2006). P300-based
detection of concealed autobiographical versus incidentally acquired
information in target and non-target paradigms. International
Journal of Psychophysiology, 60 (3), 251-259.
Rosenfeld, J. P., Cantwell, B., Nasman, V. T., Wojdac, V., Ivanov, S.,
& Mazzeri, L. (1988). A modified, event-related potential-based guilty
knowledge test. International Journal of Neuroscience, 42 (1-2),
157-161.
Rosenfeld, J. P., Hu, X., Labkovsky, E., Meixner, J., & Winograd, M. R.
(2013). Review of recent studies and issues regarding the P300-based
complex trial protocol for detection of concealed information.International Journal of Psychophysiology, 90 (2), 118-134.
Rosenfeld, J. P., Hu, X., & Pederson, K. (2012). Deception awareness
improves P300-based deception detection in concealed information tests.International Journal of Psychophysiology, 86 (1), 114-121.
Rosenfeld, J. P., Labkovsky, E., Davydova, E., Ward, A., & Rosenfeld,
L. (2017). Financial incentive does not affect P300 (in response to
certain episodic and semantic probe stimuli) in the Complex Trial
Protocol (CTP) version of the Concealed Information Test (CIT) in
detection of malingering. Psychophysiology, 54 (5), 764-772.
Rosenfeld, J. P., Labkovsky, E., Winograd, M., Lui, M. A., Vandenboom,
C., & Chedid, E. (2008). The Complex Trial Protocol (CTP): A new,
countermeasure‐resistant, accurate, P300‐based method for detection of
concealed information. Psychophysiology , 45 (6), 906-919.
Rosenfeld, J. P., Soskins, M., Bosh, G., & Ryan, A. (2004). Simple,
effective countermeasures to P300‐based tests of detection of concealed
information. Psychophysiology, 41 (2), 205-219.
Rosenfeld, J. P., Shue, E., & Singer, E. (2007). Single versus multiple
probe blocks of P300-based concealed information tests for
self-referring versus incidentally obtained information.Biological psychology, 74 (3), 396-404.
Rosenfeld, J. P., Sitar, E., Wasserman, J., & Ward, A. (2018). Moderate
financial incentive does not appear to influence the P300 Concealed
Information Test (CIT) effect in the Complex Trial Protocol (CTP)
version of the CIT in a forensic scenario, while affecting P300 peak
latencies and behavior. International Journal of Psychophysiology,
125 , 42-49.
Rouder, J. N., Speckman, P. L., Sun, D., Morey, R. D., & Iverson, G.
(2009). Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null
hypothesis. Psychonomic bulletin & review , 16 , 225-237.
Sai, L., Cheng, J., Shang, S., Fu, G., & Verschuere, B. (2023). Does
deception involve more cognitive control than truth‐telling?
Meta‐analyses of N2 and MFN ERP studies. Psychophysiology ,
e14333.
Sai, L., Lin, X., Hu, X., & Fu, G. (2014). Detecting concealed
information using feedback related event-related brain potentials.Brain and cognition, 90 , 142-150.
Sai, L., Lin, X., Rosenfeld, J. P., Sang, B., Hu, X., & Fu, G. (2016).
Novel, ERP-based, concealed information detection: Combining
recognition-based and feedback-evoked ERPs. Biological psychology,
114 , 13-22.
Sai, L., Li, H., Wang, C., Rosenfeld, J. P., Lin, X., & Fu, G. (2020).
Feedback does not influence the recognition-related P300 in a novel
concealed information test while feedback-evoked P300 shows promising
diagnostic accuracy. International Journal of
Psychophysiology , 157 , 32-41.
Sato, A., Yasuda, A., Ohira, H., Miyawaki, K., Nishikawa, M., Kumano,
H., & Kuboki, T. (2005). Effects of value and reward magnitude on
feedback negativity and P300. Neuroreport, 16 (4), 407-411.
Tacikowski, P., Cygan, H. B., & Nowicka, A. (2014). Neural correlates
of own and close-other’s name recognition: ERP evidence. Frontiers
in human neuroscience, 8 , 194.
Verschuere, B., Crombez, G., Koster, E. H., & De Clercq, A. (2007).
Antisociality, underarousal and the validity of the Concealed
Information Polygraph Test. Biological psychology, 74 (3),
309-318.
Winograd, M. R., & Rosenfeld, J. P. (2011). Mock crime application of
the Complex Trial Protocol (CTP) P300-based concealed information test.Psychophysiology, 48 (2), 155-161.
Xu, Q., Shen, Q., Chen, P., Ma, Q., Sun, D., & Pan, Y. (2011). How an
uncertain cue modulates subsequent monetary outcome evaluation: an ERP
study. Neuroscience letters, 505 (2), 200-204.
Yeung, N., & Sanfey, A. G. (2004). Independent coding of reward
magnitude and valence in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience,
24 (28), 6258-6264.
Zheng, J., Cheng, J., Wang, C., Lin, X., Fu, G., & Sai, L. (2022). The
effect of mental countermeasures on a novel brain‐based feedback
concealed information test. Human Brain Mapping , 43 (9),
2771-2781.