References
Abutalebi, J., Della Rosa, P. A., Green, D. W., Hernandez, M., Scifo, P., Keim, R., Cappa, S. F., & Costa, A. (2012). Bilingualism tunes the anterior cingulate cortex for conflict monitoring. Cerebral Cortex22 (9), 2076–2086. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr287
Adler, R. M., Valdés Kroff, J. R., & Novick, J. M. (2020). Does Integrating a Code-Switch During Comprehension Engage Cognitive Control? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition46 (4), 741–759. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000755
Avital-Cohen, R., & Tsal, Y. (2016). Top-Down Processes Override Bottom-Up Interference in the Flanker Task. Psychological Science27 (5), 651–658. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616631737
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal.Journal of Memory and Language , 68 (3), 255–278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001
Bates, D., Kliegl, R., Vasishth, S., & Baayen, H. (2015). Parsimonious mixed models. arXiv preprint arXiv:1506.04967 .
Bartholow, B. D., Pearson, M. A., Dickter, C. L., Sher, K. J., Fabiani, M., & Gratton, G. (2005). Strategic control and medial frontal negativity: Beyond errors and response conflict. Psychophysiology42 (1), 33–42. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00258.x
Berken, J. A., Chai, X., Chen, J.-K., Gracco, V. L., & Klein, D. (2016). Effects of early and late bilingualism on resting-state functional connectivity. The Journal of Neuroscience36 (4), 1165–1172. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1960-15.2016
Bialystok, E. (2017). The Bilingual Adaptation: How Minds Accommodate Experience. Psychological Bulletin143 (3), 233–262. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000099
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Grady, C., Chau, W., Ishii, R., Gunji, A., & Pantev, C. (2005). Effect of bilingualism on cognitive control in the Simon task: Evidence from MEG. NeuroImage, 24 (1), 40–49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.044
Blanco-Elorrieta, E., & Pylkkänen, L. (2016). Bilingual language control in perception versus action: MEG reveals comprehension control mechanisms in anterior cingulate cortex and domain-general control of production in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience36 (2), 290–301. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2597-15.2016
Blanco-Elorrieta, E., & Pylkkänen, L. (2017). Bilingual language switching in the laboratory versus in the wild: The spatiotemporal dynamics of adaptive language control. The Journal of Neuroscience37 (37), 9022–9036. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0553-17.2017
Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological review108 (3), 624. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624
Botvinick, M., Nystrom, L. E., Fissell, K., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (1999). Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex. Nature402 (6758), 179–181. https://doi.org/10.1038/46035
Brunner, C., Delorme, A. & Makeig, S. (2013). Eeglab: An open source Matlab toolbox for electrophysiological research. Biomedical Engineering / Biomedizinische Technik58 (SI-1-Track-G), 000010151520134182. https://doi.org/10.1515/bmt-2013-4182
Clayson, P. E., & Larson, M. J. (2011). Conflict adaptation and sequential trial effects: Support for the conflict monitoring theory.Neuropsychologia , 49 (7), 1953–1961. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.03.023
Christoffels, I. K., Firk, C., & Schiller, N. O. (2007). Bilingual language control: An event-related brain potential study. Brain Research , 1147 , 192–208. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2007.01.137
Coderre, E. L., & van Heuven, W. J. B. (2014). The effect of script similarity on executive control in bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology , 5 , 1070–1070. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01070
Cosme, D., & Lopez, R. B. (2020). Neural indicators of food cue reactivity, regulation, and valuation and their associations with body composition and daily eating behavior. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience , 18 (1). https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsaa155
Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2009). On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition , 113 (2), 135–149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.001
de Bruin, A., Roelofs, A., Dijkstra, T., & FitzPatrick, I. (2014). Domain-general inhibition areas of the brain are involved in language switching: fMRI evidence from trilingual speakers. NeuroImage, 90 , 348–359. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.049
Declerck, M., Grainger, J., Koch, I., & Philipp, A. M. (2017). Is language control just a form of executive control? Evidence for overlapping processes in language switching and task switching.Journal of Memory and Language , 95 , 138–145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2017.03.005
Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods , 134 (1), 9–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009
Dijkstra, T., & van Heuven, W. J. B. (2002). The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: From identification to decision.Bilingualism (Cambridge, England) , 5 (3), 175–197. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728902003012
Di Russo, F., Martínez, A., & Hillyard, S. A. (2003). Source analysis of event-related cortical activity during visuo-spatial attention.Cerebral Cortex , 13 (5), 486–499. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/13.5.486.
Donchin, E., & Coles, M. G. H. (1998). Context updating and the P300.The Behavioral and Brain Sciences , 21 (1), 152–154. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X98230950
Duncan‐Johnson, C. C., & Donchin, E. (1977). On quantifying surprise: The variation of event‐related potentials with subjective probability. Psychophysiology14 (5), 456-467. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1977.tb01312.x
D’Souza, D., & D’Souza, H. (2016). Bilingual Language Control Mechanisms in Anterior Cingulate Cortex and Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex: A Developmental Perspective. The Journal of Neuroscience ,36 (20), 5434–5436. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0798-16.2016
Emmorey, K., Luk, G., Pyers, J. E., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The source of enhanced cognitive control in bilinguals: Evidence from bimodal bilinguals. Psychological Science , 19 (12), 1201–1206. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02224.x
Engemann, D. A., Kozynets, O., Sabbagh, D., Lemaître, G., Varoquaux, G., Liem, F., & Gramfort, A. (2020). Combining magnetoencephalography with magnetic resonance imaging enhances learning of surrogate-biomarkers.eLife , 9 . https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.54055
Flournoy, J. C., Vijayakumar, N., Cheng, T. W., Cosme, D., Flannery, J. E., & Pfeifer, J. H. (2020). Improving practices and inferences in developmental cognitive neuroscience. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience , 45 , 100807–100807. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100807
Folstein, J. R., & Van Petten, C. (2008). Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: A review.Psychophysiology , 45 (1), 152–170. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00602.x
Folstein, J. R., Van Petten, C., & Rose, S. A. (2008). Novelty and conflict in the categorization of complex stimuli.Psychophysiology , 45 (3), 467–479. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00628.x
Gabrieli, J., Poldrack, R. A., & Desmond, J. E. (1998). The Role of Left Prefrontal Cortex in Language and Memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS , 95 (3), 906–913. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.95.3.906
Garbin, G., Sanjuan, A., Forn, C., Bustamante, J. C., Rodriguez-Pujadas, A., Belloch, V., Hernandez, M., Costa, A., & Ávila, C. (2010). Bridging language and attention: Brain basis of the impact of bilingualism on cognitive control. NeuroImage , 53 (4), 1272–1278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.078
Geranpayeh, A. (2003). A quick review of the English Quick Placement Test. UCLESResearch Notes , 12 , 8-10.
Gilbert, C. D., & Li, W. (2013). Top-down influences on visual processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience , 14 (5), 350–363. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3476
Gilead, M., Liberman, N., & Maril, A. (2013). The language of future-thought: An fMRI study of embodiment and tense processing.NeuroImage , 65 , 267–279. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.073
Gollan, T. H., & Ferreira, V. S. (2009). Should I stay or should I switch? A cost-benefit analysis of voluntary language switching in young and aging bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 35 (3), 640–665. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014981
Gollan, T. H., Kleinman, D., & Wierenga, C. E. (2014). What’s easier: Doing what you want, or being told what to do? Cued versus voluntary language and task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , 143 (6), 2167–2195. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038006
Green, D. W. (1986). Control, activation, and resource: A framework and a model for the control of speech in bilinguals. Brain and language27 (2), 210-223. https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(86)90016-7
Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J. (2013). Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology ,25 (5), 515–530. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.796377
Hannaway, N., Opitz, B., & Sauseng, P. (2019). Exploring the bilingual advantage: manipulations of similarity and second language immersion in a Stroop task. Cognitive Neuroscience , 10 (1), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2017.1389874
Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R. M. (2011). Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive context processes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review18 , 625-658. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-011-0116-7
Hsieh, S., & Lin, Y.-C. (2014). The boundary condition for observing compensatory responses by the elderly in a flanker-task paradigm.Biological Psychology , 103 , 69–82. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.08.008
Hwang, K., Bertolero, M. A., Liu, W. B., & D’Esposito, M. (2017). The human thalamus is an integrative hub for functional brain networks.The Journal of Neuroscience , 37 (23), 5594–5607. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0067-17.2017
Jackson, G. M., Swainson, R., Cunnington, R., & Jackson, S. R. (2001). ERP correlates of executive control during repeated language switching.Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4 (2), 169–178. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728901000268
Jevtović, M., Duñabeitia, J. A., & de Bruin, A. (2020). How do bilinguals switch between languages in different interactional contexts? A comparison between voluntary and mandatory language switching.Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23 (2), 401–413. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728919000191
Jiang, J., Beck, J., Heller, K., & Egner, T. (2015). An insula-frontostriatal network mediates flexible cognitive control by adaptively predicting changing control demands. Nature Communications , 6 , 8165-. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms9165
Jiao, L., Grundy, J. G., Liu, C., & Chen, B. (2020a). Language context modulates executive control in bilinguals: Evidence from language production. Neuropsychologia , 142 , 107441–107441. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107441
Jiao, L., Liu, C., Liang, L., Plummer, P., Perfetti, C. A., & Chen, B. (2019). The contributions of language control to executive functions: From the perspective of bilingual comprehension. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006) , 72 (8), 1984–1997. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021818821601
Jiao, L., Liu, C., Bruin, A., & Chen, B. (2020b). Effects of language context on executive control in unbalanced bilinguals: An ERPs study.Psychophysiology , 57 (11), e13653. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13653
Jiao, L., Timmer, K., Liu, C., & Chen, B. (2022). The Role of Language Switching During Cross‐Talk Between Bilingual Language Control and Domain‐General Conflict Monitoring. Cognitive Science ,46 (8), e13184-n/a. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.13184
Kang, C., Fu, Y., Wu, J., Ma, F., Lu, C., & Guo, T. (2017). Short‐term language switching training tunes the neural correlates of cognitive control in bilingual language production. Human Brain Mapping38 (12), 5859-5870. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23765
Wu, J., Kang, C., Ma, F., Gao, X., & Guo, T. (2018). The influence of short-term language-switching training on the plasticity of the cognitive control mechanism in bilingual word production. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology71 (10), 2115-2128. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021817737520
Kerns, J. G. (2006). Anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex activity in an FMRI study of trial-to-trial adjustments on the Simon task.NeuroImage , 33 (1), 399–405. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.012
Kerns, J. G., Cohen, J. D., MacDonald III, A. W., Cho, R. Y., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in context. Science303 (5660). https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1089910
Kleinman, D., & Gollan, T. H. (2016). Speaking two languages for the price of one: Bypassing language control mechanisms via accessibility-driven wwitches. Psychological Science ,27 (5), 700–714. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616634633
Kopp, B., Rist, F., & Mattler, U. W. E. (1996). N200 in the flanker task as a neurobehavioral tool for investigating executive context. Psychophysiology33 (3), 282-294. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1996.tb00425.x
Kroll, J. F., Dussias, P. E., Bice, K., & Perrotti, L. (2015). Bilingualism, Mind, and Brain. Annual Review of Linguistics ,1 (1), 377–394. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-linguist-030514-124937
Kropotov, J. D., Ponomarev, V. A., Hollup, S., & Mueller, A. (2011). Dissociating action inhibition, conflict monitoring, and sensory mismatch into independent components of event related potentials in go/nogo task. NeuroImage , 57 (2), 565–575. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.04.060
Kwon, Y. H., Yoo, K., Nguyen, H., Jeong, Y., & Chun, M. M. (2021). Predicting multilingual effects on executive function and individual connectomes in children: An ABCD study. PNAS , 118 (49). https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2110811118
Linck, J. A., Schwieter, J. W., & Sunderman, G. (2012). Inhibitory control predicts language switching performance in trilingual speech production. Bilingualism (Cambridge, England) , 15 (3), 651–662. https://doi.org/10.1017/S136672891100054X
Liu, C., Jiao, L., Sun, X., & Wang, R. (2016). Immediate effect of language switch on non-proficient bilinguals’ cognitive control components. Acta Psychologica Sinica48 (5), 472. https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2016.00472
Liu, D., Xing, Z., Huang, J., Schwieter, J. W., & Liu, H. (2023a). Genetic bases of language context in bilinguals: Evidence from an EEG study. Human Brain Mapping , 44 (9), 3624–3643. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.26301
Liu, D., Schwieter, J. W., Liu, W., Mu, L., & Liu, H. (2023b). The COMT gene modulates the relationship between bilingual adaptation in executive function and decision-making: An EEG study. Cognitive Neurodynamics , 17 (4), 893–907. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11571-022-09867-2
Liu, D., Schwieter, J. W., Wang, F., Mu, L., & Liu, H. (2022a). Uncovering the effects of bilingual language control on rational decisions: An ERP study. Psychophysiology , 59 (10), e14066-n/a. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.14066
Liu, H., Li, W., Zuo, M., Wang, F., Guo, Z., & Schwieter, J. W. (2022b). Cross-task adaptation effects of bilingual language control on cognitive control: A dual-brain EEG examination of simultaneous production and comprehension. Cerebral Cortex , 32 (15), 3224–3242. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhab411
Luck, S. J., Woodman, G. F., & Vogel, E. K. (2000). Event-related potential studies of attention. Trends in cognitive sciences4 (11), 432-440. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01545-X
Makeig, S., Bell, A., Jung, T. P., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1995). Independent component analysis of electroencephalographic data. In D. Touretzky, M. Mozer, & M. Hasselmo (Eds.), Advances in neural information processing systems (pp. 145-151). NeurIPS Proceedings.
Martin, C. D., Strijkers, K., Santesteban, M., Escera, C., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Costa, A. (2013). The impact of early bilingualism on controlling a language learned late: an ERP study. Frontiers in Psychology , 4 , 815–815. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00815
Masur, P. K., & Scharkow, M. (2020). specr: Conducting and visualizing specification curve analyses.
Meir-Hasson, Y., Kinreich, S., Podlipsky, I., Hendler, T., & Intrator, N. (2014). An EEG finger-print of fMRI deep regional activation.NeuroImage, 102 , 128–141. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.004
Neuhaus, A. H., Urbanek, C., Opgen-Rhein, C., Hahn, E., Ta, T. M. T., Koehler, S., Gross, M., & Dettling, M. (2010). Event-related potentials associated with attention network test. International Journal of Psychophysiology , 76 (2), 72–79. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.02.005
Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clinical Neurophysiology118 (10), 2128–2148. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019
Rafeekh, R., & Mishra, R. K. (2021). The sensitivity to context modulates executive control: Evidence from Malayalam-English bilinguals.Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 24 (2), 358–373. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728920000528
Ramalingam, N., McManus, J. N. J., Li, W., & Gilbert, C. D. (2013). Top-down modulation of lateral interactions in visual cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33 (5), 1773–1789. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3825-12.2013
Rouder, J. N., & King, J. W. (2003). Flanker and negative flanker effects in letter identification. Perception & Psychophysics65 (2), 287–297. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194800
Simonsohn, U., Simmons, J. P., & Nelson, L. D. (2020). Specification curve analysis. Nature Human Behaviour4 (11), 1208–1214. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-0912-z
Smith, J. L., Johnstone, S. J., & Barry, R. J. (2007). Response priming in the go/nogo task: The N1 reflects neither inhibition nor conflict.Clinical Neurophysiology, 118 (2), 343–355. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2006.09.027
Snodgrass, J. G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of experimental psychology: Human learning and memory6 (2), 174-215. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.6.2.174
Stein, M., Winkler, C., Kaiser, A., & Dierks, T. (2014). Structural brain changes related to bilingualism: does immersion make a difference?Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1116–1116. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01116
Teufel, C., & Nanay, B. (2017). How to (and how not to) think about top-down influences on visual perception. Consciousness and Cognition , 47 , 17–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.05.008
Thierry, G., & Wu, Y. J. (2007). Brain potentials reveal unconscious translation during foreign-language comprehension. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS , 104 (30), 12530–12535. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0609927104
Timmer, K., Costa, A., & Wodniecka, Z. (2021). The source of attention modulations in bilingual language contexts. Brain and Language223 , 105040–105040. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2021.105040
van Veen, V., & Carter, C. S. (2002). The anterior cingulate as a conflict monitor: fMRI and ERP studies. Physiology & Behavior ,77 (4), 477–482. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0031-9384(02)00930-7
Verhoef, K. M. W., Roelofs, A., & Chwilla, D. J. (2010). Electrophysiological evidence for endogenous control of attention in switching between languages in overt picture naming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 22 (8), 1832–1843. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21291
Westbrook, A., & Braver, T. S. (2015). Cognitive effort: A neuroeconomic approach. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience , 15 (2), 395–415. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-015-0334-y.
Whorf, B. L., Carroll, J. B., Levinson, S. C., & Lee, P. (2012).Language, thought, and reality: Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf (2nd edition). MIT Press.
Wu, Y. J., & Thierry, G. (2013). Fast modulation of executive function by language context in bilinguals. The Journal of Neuroscience ,33 (33), 13533–13537. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4760-12.2013
Wu, J., Yang, J., Chen, M., Li, S., Zhang, Z., Kang, C., Ding, G., & Guo, T. (2019). Brain network reconfiguration for language and domain-general cognitive control in bilinguals. NeuroImage ,199 , 454–465. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.06.022
Yeo, B. T. T., Krienen, F. M., Eickhoff, S. B., Yaakub, S. N., Fox, P. T., Buckner, R. L., Asplund, C. L., & Chee, M. W. L. (2016). Functional specialization and flexibility in human association cortex.Cerebral Cortex , 26 (1), 465–465. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhv260
Yuan, Q., Li, H., Du, B., Dang, Q., Chang, Q., Zhang, Z., Zhang, M., Ding, G., Lu, C., & Guo, T. (2022). The cerebellum and cognition: further evidence for its role in language control. Cerebral Cortex , 33 (1), 35–49. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhac051
Yuan, Q., Ma, F., Zhang, M., Chen, M., Zhang, Z., Wu, J., Lu, C., & Guo, T. (2021). Neural interaction between language control and cognitive control: Evidence from cross-task adaptation.Behavioural Brain Research , 401 , 113086–113086. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2020.113086
Zhang, Q., & Yang, Y. (2003). The determiners of picture-naming latency. Acta Psychologica Sinica35 (04), 447.
Zhu, J. D., Blanco-Elorrieta, E., Sun, Y., Szakay, A., & Sowman, P. F. (2022). Natural vs forced language switching: Free selection and consistent language use eliminate significant performance costs and cognitive demands in the brain. NeuroImage, 247 , 118797–118797. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118797